pretend play

Researchers say there is evidence infants are exploring the world of make-believe before they鈥檙e even aware they are doing it

10 November 2022

7

A new study exploring the origins of pretend play suggests infants perform interactional patterns with elements of pretence a lot earlier than thought. 

Pretend play is often considered a developmental landmark, being linked to emotion regulation, language skills, cognitive reasoning, and problem-solving. It is widely accepted a child begins participating in make-believe activities when they have developed the capacity to recognise they are doing it and, in most cases, studies focus on infants who are somewhat verbal.

However, by the 小蓝视频 and Lund University, Sweden, has drawn connections between pretence and a child鈥檚 early on playful interactions, such as clowning and teasing.

Before they become directors of imaginary worlds, they start off small by being actors and performing something unconventional

Iris Nomikou, 小蓝视频鈥檚 Department of Psychology

Iris Nomikou, from the 小蓝视频鈥檚 Department of Psychology, said: 鈥淲e often see children using props that stand for something else, like a stick as a sword or a bowl as a hat. But before they become directors of imaginary worlds, they start off small by being actors and performing something unconventional.

鈥淭his can be pulling funny facial expressions, making unnatural noises, and even infant acting - like they鈥檙e going to do something when they鈥檙e not - to get the attention of a parent, caregiver or friend.鈥

Pretend play is usually defined as an activity with a symbolic character, in which a signifier (e.g. a banana) is used to represent the meaning of a signified (e.g. a telephone). But the paper, published in the , says variations exist in its quality, emergence and developmental progress across different contexts and cultures.

The authors argue that pretence should stop being defined as an end-product of cognitive development, and instead an interpersonal one. Its origins can then be moved to much earlier in infancy than was originally thought possible.

What these earlier and later forms of actions have in common most, is that they are spaces in which infants and children can construct and explore different kinds of realities with meaningful others

Valentina Fantasia, Department of Philosophy and Cognitive Science at Lund University

, added: 鈥淭here is observational evidence of a child as young as eight months old pretending to give an object to someone, then withdrawing it as soon as they reach out. 

鈥淢ost parents and caregivers have experienced these types of interactions, but still not much attention has been dedicated to investigating their broadest developmental impact or the continuity that exists with pretend play. 

鈥淲hat these earlier and later forms of actions have in common most, is that they are spaces in which infants and children can construct and explore different kinds of realities with meaningful others.鈥

The study recommends further observation of early form pretence to see how pretend play can be encouraged from a younger age. If more attention is given to the role of early caregiver-infant interactions, from parents reading a book in a character鈥檚 voice to playing peek-a-boo, it allows a child to 鈥榓ct their part鈥 from day one of their lives.